国际米兰对阵科莫 - learning /taxonomy/subjects/learning en 'Reductive' models of wellbeing education risk failing children, researchers warn /research/news/reductive-models-of-wellbeing-education-risk-failing-children-unless-improved-researchers-warn <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/picture-1.jpg?itok=d3W119Kt" alt="Teacher speaking with students" title="Teacher speaking with students, Credit: None" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>In a new compendium of academic analysis, researchers argue that despite decades of investment in 鈥榩ositive education鈥 鈥 such as programmes to teach children <a href="https://www.theguardian.com/society/2017/mar/12/schools-to-trial-happiness-lessons-for-eight-year-olds">happiness and mindfulness</a> 鈥 schools still lack a proper framework for cultivating pupil wellbeing.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The critique appears in Wellbeing and Schooling, a book launched on 21 June. It compiles work by members of the <a href="https://eera-ecer.de/networks/nw08/">European Health and Wellbeing Education research network</a>, which engages specialists from around the world.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>It argues that many education systems, including in the UK, treat wellbeing education reductively, generally viewing it as a means to drive up attainment. It links this viewpoint to the prevalence of one-size-fits-all models such as the 鈥榟appiness agenda鈥: a sequence of initiatives which have tried to promote 鈥<a href="https://actionforhappiness.org/toolkit-for-schools">happier living</a>鈥 in British schools in recent years. These typically focus on training pupils to adopt a positive mindset. Commonly recommended methods include keeping gratitude journals and recording happy memories.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The authors suggest that such approaches, while useful, have limited impact. Instead, they say wellbeing should be 鈥渁n educational goal in its own right鈥. Fulfilling that requires a more nuanced approach, in which pupils engage purposefully with the circumstances that influence their wellbeing, as well as their own feelings.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Their book presents various examples from around the world of how this has been achieved. They range from system-wide strategies, such as the use of 鈥<a href="https://www.citizensinformation.ie/en/education/primary_and_post_primary_education/going_to_post_primary_school/transition_year.html#la82be">Transition Years</a>鈥 in Ireland and South Korea; to small-scale programmes and pilot studies, such as a project co-created by parents and teachers in New Zealand which drew on indigenous Maori heritage.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Wellbeing is typically conceptualised as having two dimensions: a 鈥榟edonic鈥 aspect, which refers to feelings and personal satisfaction, and a 鈥榚udaimonic鈥 aspect; a sense of meaningful purpose. Ros McLellan, an Associate Professor at the Faculty of Education, 国际米兰对阵科莫, who co-edited the book, said most wellbeing education focused only on the hedonic dimension.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淚f education doesn鈥檛 also guide children towards doing things that they find worthwhile and meaningful, we鈥檙e failing them,鈥 McLellan said. 鈥淲e limit their prospects of becoming successful, flourishing citizens. Life satisfaction is also more complex than we tend to acknowledge. It鈥檚 about dealing with both positive and negative experiences. Just running lessons on how to be happy won鈥檛 work. At worst, it risks making children who aren鈥檛 happy feel as if that鈥檚 their own fault.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>There is some evidence that wellbeing education, as presently realised, is failing to cut through. The Children鈥檚 Society <a href="https://www.childrenssociety.org.uk/good-childhood">has reported</a> that 306,000 10 to 15-year-olds are unhappy with their lives, while one in eight feels under pressure at school. <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/02673843.2019.1596823">Other research</a> on pupil stress raises questions about why the standard <a href="https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/relationships-education-relationships-and-sex-education-rse-and-health-education/physical-health-and-mental-wellbeing-primary-and-secondary">policy justification</a> for wellbeing education remains the 鈥減ositive impact on behaviour and attainment鈥.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><a href="https://link-springer-com.ezp.lib.cam.ac.uk/chapter/10.1007/978-3-030-95205-1_2">One chapter</a> in the book, co-authored by Professor Venka Simovska, from Aarhus University, Denmark (together with Catriona O`Toole), <a href="https://www.au.dk/en/9ca7edf2-b19f-4cdc-8b23-263227832d36">raises concerns</a> that the happiness agenda overlooks the fact that some pupils inevitably find it difficult to suppress negative emotions, and fails to reflect whether focusing solely on positive feelings is beneficial for wellbeing.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淪tudents are faced with ever-increasing exhortations to be upbeat, to persist in the face of challenges, to display a growth mindset, to be enterprising and resilient,鈥 the researchers write. 鈥淩epeated over time, this can give rise to an atmosphere of toxic positivity, particularly for those whose life experiences and living conditions do not lend themselves to feelings of cheery enthusiasm.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>As an alternative, they point to the recent revival in Scandinavia and elsewhere of Bildung, a German educational philosophy that links independent personal development to wider notions of purpose and social responsibility.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Informed by this tradition, schools in Denmark have applied a participatory and action-oriented pedagogical model to health and wellbeing education. The model starts by encouraging students to discuss an issue, for example how they feel when in school, then the teacher guides the students to critically explore the dynamics 鈥 either within their school or beyond 鈥 which might influence this, and envision creative possibilities for positive transformation.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Teachers and students together then develop programmes which address these structural influences and try to bring about change. The result has been school-level projects that address issues such as social inequality, marginalisation and discrimination related to health and wellbeing. 鈥淥ne could describe it as a form of citizenship education, but focused on school-related or wider societal determinants of wellbeing,鈥 Simovska said.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The book also underlines the need to avoid generic, often Eurocentric, responses to promoting wellbeing in school, to consider complexities of culturally sensitive and multicultural environments, and to focus on both local circumstances and the specific needs of different demographic groups.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><a href="https://link-springer-com.ezp.lib.cam.ac.uk/chapter/10.1007/978-3-030-95205-1_10">One chapter</a> examines Ireland鈥檚 use of an optional 鈥楾ransition Year鈥, in which students focus on developmental activities and work experience, partly to help them become more 鈥渇ulfilled citizens鈥. This has inspired the introduction of <a href="https://www.krivet.re.kr/eng/eu/zc/euZ_prA.jsp?dv=G&amp;gn=M16%7CM160000008%7C1">鈥楩ree Years鈥 in South Korea</a>. The South Korean model, however, necessarily involved adaptations to address local issues. Most obviously, Free Years, introduced in 2013, are compulsory, reflecting deep nationwide concerns in South Korea 鈥渁bout student wellbeing and stress in a high-stakes academic environment鈥 鈥 manifest in rising rates of school violence and youth suicide.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Another chapter reports how researchers at the University of Canterbury, Christchurch, orchestrated a series of w膩nanga 鈥 traditional Maori knowledge-sharing gatherings 鈥 for parents and teachers on New Zealand鈥檚 South Island, to examine local communities鈥 ideas and priorities for wellbeing.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Teachers used these to devise effective strategies for helping pupils to develop positive relationships and express emotions, often drawing on Maori culture. In one particularly touching example, a primary school teacher introduced a symbolic Maori Stone into her classroom, to which children could 鈥榯ransfer鈥 thoughts and feelings. She found it became a useful tool for working through moments of unrest and disagreement.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>McLellan believes such cases illustrate how a more nuanced approach to wellbeing education is particularly feasible in primary settings. 鈥淎rguably, it鈥檚 important we start as young as we can,鈥 she said. 鈥淭he examples in the book also show what amazing things teachers and schools can do, if we give them the resources and space to implement really effective, comprehensive, socio-ecological and culturally sensitive wellbeing education.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em>Wellbeing and Schooling: Cross Cultural and Cross Disciplinary Perspectives</em> is published by <a href="https://link.springer.com/book/10.1007/978-3-030-95205-1">Springer</a>, within the book series of the European Educational Research Association鈥檚 book series titled Transdisciplinary Perspectives in Educational research. The book will be launched at an event on 21 June.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>An improved vision for wellbeing education should replace the over-simplistic approaches currently employed in many schools, such as happiness lessons, which risk creating an 鈥渁tmosphere of toxic positivity鈥 for pupils, experts say.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">If education doesn鈥檛 also guide children towards doing things that they find worthwhile and meaningful, we鈥檙e failing them</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Ros McLellan</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Teacher speaking with students</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. Images, including our videos, are Copyright 漏国际米兰对阵科莫 and licensors/contributors as identified.聽 All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways 鈥 as here, on our <a href="/">main website</a> under its <a href="/about-this-site/terms-and-conditions">Terms and conditions</a>, and on a <a href="/about-this-site/connect-with-us">range of channels including social media</a> that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Tue, 21 Jun 2022 00:11:28 +0000 tdk25 232771 at Students who self-identify as multilingual perform better at GCSE /research/news/students-who-self-identify-as-multilingual-perform-better-at-gcse <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/jacqueline-brandwayn-s8msj5vzhxq-unsplash_0.jpg?itok=gL5B2XAK" alt="Saying goodbye" title="Saying goodbye, Credit: Jacqueline Brandwyn via Unsplash" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/full/10.1080/15348458.2021.1986397">The study</a>, of just over 800 pupils in England, found a positive relationship between GCSE scores and 鈥榤ultilingual identity鈥: a reference to whether pupils felt a personal connection with other languages through knowledge and use. Those who self-identified as multilingual typically outperformed their peers not just in subjects such as French and Spanish, but in non-language subjects including maths, geography and science. This applied whether or not they actually spoke a second language fluently.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Perhaps surprisingly, however, not all pupils who were officially described by their schools as having 鈥楨nglish as a Second Language鈥 (EAL) thought of themselves as multilingual, even though the term is used by schools and Government as a proxy for multilingualism. Correspondingly, these pupils did not necessarily perform better (or worse) as a group at GCSE than their non-EAL peers.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The results indicate that encouraging pupils to identify with languages and to value different styles of communication could help them to develop a mindset that supports academic progress overall.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><a href="https://news.educ.cam.ac.uk/cultivating-multilingual-identities-could-reverse-crisis">Other recent research</a> has argued for broadening the scope of language lessons so that, as well as studying vocabulary and grammar, pupils explore the importance of languages and their significance for their own lives. This new study was the first, however, to examine the relationship between multilingual identity and attainment. It was led by academics at the 国际米兰对阵科莫 and the findings are published in the Journal of Language, Identity and Education.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Dee Rutgers, a Research Associate at the Faculty of Education, 国际米兰对阵科莫, said: 鈥淭he evidence suggests that the more multilingual you consider yourself to be, the higher your GCSE scores. While we need to understand more about why that relationship exists, it may be that children who see themselves as multilingual have a sort of 鈥榞rowth mindset鈥 which impacts on wider attainment.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Linda Fisher, Reader in Languages Education at the 国际米兰对阵科莫, said: 鈥淭here could be a strong case for helping children who think that they can鈥檛 鈥榙o鈥 languages to recognise that we all use a range of communication tools, and that learning a language is simply adding to that range. This may influence attitude and self-belief, which is directly relevant to learning at school. In other words, what you think you are may be more important than what others say you are.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The study鈥檚 authors argue that being multilingual means far more than the <a href="https://www.gov.uk/government/statistics/schools-pupils-and-their-characteristics-january-2020">official EAL definition</a> of being 鈥榚xposed to a language at home that is known or believed to be other than English鈥. They suggest that even young people who see themselves as monolingual possess a 鈥榬epertoire鈥 of communication. For example, they may use different dialects, pick up words and phrases on holiday, know sign language, or understand other types of 鈥榣anguage鈥 such as computer code.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The study involved 818 Year-11 pupils at five secondary schools in South East England. As well as establishing whether pupils were officially registered as EAL or non-EAL, the researchers asked each pupil if they personally identified as such. Separately, each pupil was asked to plot where they saw themselves on a 0-100 scale, where 0 represented 鈥榤onolingual鈥 and 100 鈥榤ultilingual鈥. This data was compared with their GCSE results in nine subjects.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Students who spoke a second language at home did not always personally identify either as EAL or multilingual. Conversely, pupils who saw themselves as multilingual were not always those earmarked by the school as having English as an additional language.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淭he fact that these terms didn鈥檛 correlate more closely is surprising considering that they are all supposedly measuring the same thing,鈥 Rutgers said. 鈥淛ust having experience of other languages clearly doesn鈥檛 necessarily translate into a multilingual identity because the experience may not be valued by the student.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>School-reported EAL status had no impact on GCSE results, although pupils who self-identified as EAL generally did better than their peers in modern languages. Those who considered themselves 鈥榤ultilingual鈥 on the 0-100 scale, however, performed better academically across the board.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The strength of this relationship varied between subjects and was, again, particularly pronounced in modern languages. In all nine GCSE subjects assessed, however, each point increase on the monolingual-to-multilingual scale was associated with a fractional rise in pupils鈥 exam scores.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>For example: a one-point increase was found to correspond to 0.012 of a grade in Science, and 0.011 of a grade in Geography. Students who consider themselves very multilingual would, by this measure, typically score a full grade higher than those who consider themselves monolingual. Positively identifying as multilingual could often therefore be enough to push students who would otherwise fall slightly short of a certain grade up to the next level.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The findings appear to indicate that the positive mentality and self-belief which typically develops among pupils with a multilingual identity has spill-over benefits for their wider education. The authors add that this could be cultivated in languages classrooms: for example, by exposing young people to learning programmes that explore different types of language and dialect, or encouraging them to think about how languages shape their lives both inside and outside school.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淭oo often we think about other languages as something that we don鈥檛 need to know, or as difficult to learn,鈥 Fisher said. 鈥淭hese findings suggest that if pupils were encouraged to see themselves as active and capable language learners, it could have a really positive impact on their wider progress at school.鈥</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Young people who consider themselves 鈥榤ultilingual鈥 tend to perform better across a wide range of subjects at school, regardless of whether they are actually fluent in another language, new research shows.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">If pupils were encouraged to see themselves as active and capable language learners, it could have a really positive impact on their wider progress at school.</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Linda Fisher</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://unsplash.com/photos/happy-birthday-greeting-card-lot-S8MSj5VzHxQ" target="_blank">Jacqueline Brandwyn via Unsplash</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Saying goodbye</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. Images, including our videos, are Copyright 漏国际米兰对阵科莫 and licensors/contributors as identified.聽 All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways 鈥 as here, on our <a href="/">main website</a> under its <a href="/about-this-site/terms-and-conditions">Terms and conditions</a>, and on a <a href="/about-this-site/connect-with-us">range of channels including social media</a> that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Thu, 11 Nov 2021 00:26:41 +0000 tdk25 228161 at "It鈥檚 almost as if they don鈥檛 exist鈥: Education policy fails to account for PMLD learners /research/news/its-almost-as-if-they-dont-exist-education-policy-fails-to-account-for-pmld-learners <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/pmdl.jpg?itok=mqRSQ7jq" alt="" title="Teacher with PDML pupil, Credit: Getty via Routledge" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>The research, which is published in a book launched on Tuesday 9 November, found that the key piece of statutory guidance underpinning education for PMLD learners 鈥 the <a href="https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/398815/SEND_Code_of_Practice_January_2015.pdf">Special Needs and Disabilities (SEND) Code of Practice (2015)</a> 鈥 indicates that teachers should prepare them for a future that involves independent living, possible further education and employment.</p> <p>Researchers argue that these are highly unlikely to represent realistic goals for most children with PMLD. Broadly, PMLD describes people with a combination of very severe learning difficulties, sensory impairments, physical disabilities, complex medical conditions, and challenging behaviours. Most require very high levels of care and support throughout their lives, including with tasks such as washing and eating.</p> <p>The study also analysed other key health and social care policy documents on which support for children with PMLD is meant to be based. It found that these often make similarly unrealistic assumptions: 鈥渂ecause judgements are based on the experiences and values of the policy-makers, because all types and levels of disability are seen as effectively the same, and because people with PMLD tend to be viewed as non-contributors to society鈥.</p> <p>For example, <a href="https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/215891/dh_122387.pdf">Valuing People Now</a>, a Government policy document published in 2009, states that people with learning difficulties 鈥 apparently including those with PMLD 鈥 鈥渟hould be supported to pay taxes, vote [and] do jury duty鈥.</p> <p>The book, Enhancing Wellbeing and Independence for Young People with Profound and Multiple Learning Difficulties, combines this policy analysis with the findings from staff surveys at more than 110 special schools in 20 countries, including 52 of around 300 schools that teach PMLD pupils in the UK.</p> <p>It was co-authored by Andrew Colley, a former special education teacher and lecturer, who did the research as part of a Masters Degree at the Faculty of Education, 国际米兰对阵科莫; and Julie Tilbury, Lead Teacher for children with PMLD at Chailey Heritage School, East Sussex.</p> <p>Their findings highlight the outstanding practice of professionals working with pupils with PMLD pupils, but also suggest that teachers rarely refer to the existing policy guidance except when completing official documents. Asked if they felt that the SEND Code of Practice took account of learners with PMLD, teachers commented: 鈥渋t doesn鈥檛鈥, 鈥渘ot at all鈥 and 鈥渋t鈥檚 almost as if they don鈥檛 exist鈥.</p> <p>鈥淭he way wellbeing and independence are defined in policy doesn鈥檛 appear to support these learners and ends up excluding them because of the complexity of their disability,鈥 Colley said.</p> <p>鈥淢ost of the guidance that exists assumes their education can be rooted in neurotypical expectations about employment or making an economic contribution when the reality is they will probably never be able to work. The policy covers children with PMLD, but doesn鈥檛 cater for them. We need a completely different kind of social contract for these young people.鈥</p> <p>There are around 11,000 learners with PMLD in English schools, and an estimated 75,000 people of all ages with PMLD in the UK. Previous research has attempted to identify what 鈥榳ellbeing鈥 and 鈥榠ndependence鈥 should mean for these individuals. In general, it recommends that schools should focus on helping them to live with dignity, form social and emotional relationships, stay healthy and active, and communicate 鈥 which for people with PMLD often involves unconventional styles of communication such as blinking and physical gestures.</p> <p>Contrastingly, the SEND Code of Practice, which makes just one reference to pupils with PMLD in 287 pages, states: 鈥淲ith high aspirations and the right support the vast majority of children and young people can go on to achieve successful long-term outcomes in adult life,鈥 before referring to 鈥渉igher education and/or employment鈥 and 鈥渋ndependent living鈥 as examples.</p> <p>Many practitioners working with PMLD learners treat the Code as an irrelevance, the researchers found. As much as possible, teachers create learning programmes which respond to the needs of each individual. In line with the recommendations of specialists, this often means that lessons prioritise the enhancement of wellbeing and health, communication, and the development of basic skills such as washing, eating, and independent movement. 鈥淭here is fantastic work going on in schools, but it is completely separate from what policy dictates,鈥 Colley said.</p> <p>Despite the efforts of education professionals, the study also highlights the limited opportunities learners with PMLD have to engage with their wider communities. 80% of UK teachers mostly or completely agreed with the statement: 鈥渢he social life of someone with PMLD is largely focused on their family or school鈥. Almost 50% felt that families with a member who has PMLD 鈥渓ive isolated and unfulfilled lives鈥.</p> <p>The research calls for a different type of policy framework for learners with PMLD which focuses on helping them to become happy, fulfilled and empowered adults, with a sense of belonging rooted in warm and trusting relationships.</p> <p>Colley added: 鈥淭o demand that their education should lead to independence in a conventional sense stigmatises their condition, as well as their families.聽Just because pupils with PMLD are unlikely to work or own a house doesn鈥檛 make them any less worthy of our attention as human beings.鈥</p> <p>鈥淎ddressing this also gives us an opportunity to think differently about what education for all young people really means, beyond the perspective of employment or academic attainment. A really inclusive education system that takes PMLD learners into account demands that we look for something more for everyone.鈥</p> <p>Enhancing Wellbeing and Independence for Young People with Profound and Multiple Learning Difficulties is published by Routledge.</p> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>The policy framework that supposedly guides education for pupils with Profound and Multiple Learning Disabilities (PMLD) is setting expectations and goals which are often completely at odds with their capabilities and lives, a study says.</p> </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">We need a completely different kind of social contract for these young people</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Andrew Colley</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/" target="_blank">Getty via Routledge</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Teacher with PDML pupil</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br /> The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. Images, including our videos, are Copyright 漏国际米兰对阵科莫 and licensors/contributors as identified.聽 All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways 鈥 as here, on our <a href="/">main website</a> under its <a href="/about-this-site/terms-and-conditions">Terms and conditions</a>, and on a <a href="/about-this-site/connect-with-us">range of channels including social media</a> that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.</p> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Tue, 09 Nov 2021 11:41:01 +0000 tdk25 228111 at Extra classroom time may do little to help pupils recover lost learning after COVID-19 /research/news/extra-classroom-time-may-do-little-to-help-pupils-recover-lost-learning-after-covid-19 <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/school_1.jpg?itok=TsypJhOf" alt="School " title="School , Credit: Jeswin Thomas via Unsplash" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>The 国际米兰对阵科莫 analysis used five years of Government data, collected from more than 2,800 schools in England, to estimate the likely impact of additional classroom instruction on academic progress, as measured at GCSE.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>It found that even substantial increases in classroom teaching time would likely only lead to small improvements. For example, extending Year 11 pupils鈥 classroom time by one hour per class, in English or maths, was associated with an increase of 0.12 and 0.18 in a school鈥檚 鈥榲alue-added鈥 score 鈥 a standard progress measure. This increase appears small, considering that most of the schools in the study had scores ranging between 994 and 1006.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The research also investigated the likely impact for disadvantaged pupils, whose education has been hardest hit by school closures. In keeping with the overall results, it again found that more of the same teaching was likely to do relatively little to improve academic outcomes.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The study was undertaken by Vaughan Connolly, a doctoral researcher at the Faculty of Education, 国际米兰对阵科莫. His paper reporting the findings, published in the聽<em><a href="https://www.scienceopen.com/hosted-document?doi=10.14324/LRE.19.1.17">London Review of Education</a></em>, suggests that long-term plans to recoup lost learning may be better off focusing on maximising the value of the existing school day, rather than extending it.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淪imply keeping all students in school for longer, in order to do more maths or more English, probably won鈥檛 improve results much; nor is it likely to narrow the attainment gap for those who have missed out the most,鈥 Connolly said.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淭his evidence suggests that re-evaluating how time is used in schools 鈥 for example, by trimming subject time and replacing it with sessions focusing on 鈥榣earning to learn鈥 skills 鈥 could make a bigger difference. Quality is going to matter much more than quantity in the long run.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>One possible reason why additional instruction time may be relatively ineffective is diminishing returns 鈥 namely, that more contact hours simply increase the burden on both teachers and pupils, preventing them from being at their best.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Potentially extending the school day has been widely discussed as one possible component of a forthcoming聽<a href="https://www.gov.uk/government/news/new-commissioner-appointed-to-oversee-education-catch-up">Government recovery plan聽for education</a>. While there is international evidence suggesting that additional teaching time only leads to small returns, there had been no large-scale study of this issue in the English school system until now.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The 国际米兰对阵科莫 study used timetable data gathered from 2,815 schools through the聽<a href="https://www.gov.uk/government/collections/statistics-school-workforce">School Workforce Census</a>聽over five years. It tracked the relationship between changes to the amount of instruction time that pupils received in English, maths, science and humanities subjects, and their academic progress.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥楶rogress鈥 was identified using schools鈥 value-added scores. The Government gathers these when pupils sit GCSEs at age 16, by comparing their actual results with predictions made after their primary school SATs at age 11.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>While the impact of additional classroom tuition on progress varied between subjects and groups, the effects were generally small. For example: one additional hour of instruction for a Year 11 class in English, science, maths, or the humanities, led to an increase in value-added scores of 0.12, 0.09, 0.18 and 0.43 respectively. 鈥楢t a practical level, this seems small, particularly when considering the cost of such time,鈥 the study notes.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>To examine the potential impact of extra classroom time on less-advantaged students, the study also assessed how far it closed the gap between the value-added scores of students on free school meals, and those of students with middle-ranking prior attainment. The results were again found to be modest. For example, an extra 59 minutes per week in English reduced the attainment gap between these groups by about 6.5%; and an extra 57 minutes per week of maths by about 8%.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The findings compare with those of the Education Endowment Foundation鈥檚 influential聽<a href="https://epi.org.uk/publications-and-research/analysis-paper-preliminary-research-findings-on-education-recovery/">Teaching and Learning toolkit</a>, which summarises international evidence on different teaching interventions and translates their effect sizes into months of progress. It suggests that increased instruction time is likely to lead to two months of progress over an academic year. This compares poorly with the results of other interventions listed in the same document.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In this context, the 国际米兰对阵科莫 study suggests that methods which focus on increasing the quality of learning in the classroom, rather than the amount of time spent there, may prove more fruitful. It echoes recommendations recently made by the聽<a href="https://epi.org.uk/publications-and-research/analysis-paper-preliminary-research-findings-on-education-recovery/">Education Policy Institute</a>聽which called for ambitious levels of investment in a wider-ranging programme of catch-up measures. The new study suggests that time could be reallocated during the school day, either to support the continuing professional development of staff, or to provide pupils with additional skills.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>It also points to聽<a href="https://bera-journals.onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1080/09585176.2015.1137778">research conducted in 2016</a>聽in which Key Stage 3 pupils鈥 test scores improved dramatically after a portion of their regular curriculum was replaced with training in metacognition 鈥 the ability to understand how to learn and reason through problems. Other studies, such as聽a project聽examining learning recovery after the 2011 earthquake in Christchurch, New Zealand, have similarly suggested that supporting schools to better match their curriculum to student needs may have greater effect than extra classroom time.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淩ather than extending the school day to offer more instruction, a successful recovery agenda may well be one that tailors support and makes room for a wider range of learning within it, in line with the recent suggestions made by the EPI,鈥 Connolly said. 鈥淚n that sense, less instructional time could actually be more. Certainly, these results suggest that giving children more of the same is unlikely to help if we want to recover what has been lost during the pandemic.鈥</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Adding extra classroom time to the school day may only result in marginal gains for pupils who have lost learning during the COVID pandemic, a study says.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Simply keeping all students in school for longer, in order to do more maths or more English, probably won鈥檛 improve results much</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Vaughan Connolly</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://unsplash.com/photos/man-in-brown-sweater-sitting-on-chair--hgJu2ykh4E" target="_blank">Jeswin Thomas via Unsplash</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">School </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. Images, including our videos, are Copyright 漏国际米兰对阵科莫 and licensors/contributors as identified.聽 All rights reserved. We make our image and video content available in a number of ways 鈥 as here, on our <a href="/">main website</a> under its <a href="/about-this-site/terms-and-conditions">Terms and conditions</a>, and on a <a href="/about-this-site/connect-with-us">range of channels including social media</a> that permit your use and sharing of our content under their respective Terms.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Fri, 28 May 2021 09:39:52 +0000 tdk25 224461 at Young children use physics, not previous rewards, to learn about tools /research/news/young-children-use-physics-not-previous-rewards-to-learn-about-tools <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/2366314902e290cf4400b.jpg?itok=puLM6w68" alt="Dominoes 3" title="Dominoes 3, Credit: Sharon Mollerus" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>The findings of the study, based on the Aesop鈥檚 fable <em>The Crow and the Pitcher</em>, help solve a debate about whether children learning to use tools are genuinely learning about physical causation or are just driven by what action previously led to a treat.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Learning about causality 鈥 about the physical rules that govern the world around us 鈥 is a crucial part of our cognitive development. From our observations and the outcome of our own actions, we build an idea 鈥 a model 鈥 of which tools are functional for particular jobs, and which are not.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>However, the information we receive isn鈥檛 always as straightforward as it should be. Sometimes outside influences mean that things that should work, don鈥檛. Similarly, sometimes things that shouldn鈥檛 work, do.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Lucy Cheke from the Department of Psychology at the 国际米兰对阵科莫 says: 鈥淚magine a situation where someone is learning about hammers. There are two hammers that they are trying out 鈥 a metal one and an inflatable one. Normally, the metal hammer would successfully drive a nail into a plank of wood, while the inflatable hammer would bounce off harmlessly.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淏ut what if your only experience of these two hammers was trying to use the metal hammer and missing the nail, but using the inflatable hammer to successfully push the nail into a large pre-drilled hole? If you鈥檙e then presented with another nail, which tool would you choose to use? The answer depends on what type of information you have taken from your learning experience.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In this situation, explains, Cheke, a learner concerned with the outcome (a 鈥榬eward鈥 learner) would learn that the inflatable hammer was the successful tool and opt to use it for later hammering. However, a learner concerned with physical forces (a 鈥榝unctionality鈥 learner) would learn that the metal hammer produced a percussive force, albeit in the wrong place, and that the inflatable hammer did not, and would therefore opt for the metal hammer.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Now, in a study published in the open access journal <em>PLOS ONE</em>, Dr Cheke and colleagues investigated what kind of information children extract from situations where the relevant physical characteristics of a potential tool are observable, but often at odds with whether the use of that tool in practice achieved the desired goal.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The researchers presented children aged 4-11 with a task through which they must retrieve a floating token to earn sticker rewards. Each time, the children were presented with a container of water and a set of tools to use to raise the level. This experiment is based on one of the most famous Aesop鈥檚 fables, where a thirty crow drops stones into a pitcher to get to the water.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In this test, some of the tools were 鈥榝unctional鈥 and some 鈥榥on-functional鈥. Functional tools were those that, if dropped into a standard container, would sink, raising the water level and bringing the token within reach; non-functional tools were those that would not do so, for example because they floated.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>However, sometimes the children used functional tools to attempt to raise the level in a leaking container 鈥 in this context, the water would never rise high enough to bring the token within reach, no matter how functional the tool used.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>At other times, the children were successful in retrieving the reward despite using a non-functional tool; for example, when using a water container that self-fills through an inlet pipe, it doesn鈥檛 matter whether the tool is functional as the water is rising anyway.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>After these learning sessions, the researchers presented the children with a 鈥榮tandard鈥 water container and a series of choices between different tools. From the pattern of these choices the researchers could calculate what type of information was most influential on children鈥檚 decision-making: reward or function.聽</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淎 child doesn鈥檛 have to know the precise rules of physics that allow a tool to work to have a feeling of whether or not it should work,鈥 says Elsa Loissel, co-first author of the study. 鈥淪o, we can look at whether a child鈥檚 decision making is guided by principles of physics without requiring them to explicitly understand the physics itself.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淲e expected older children, who might have a rudimentary understanding of physical forces, to choose according to function, while younger children would be expected to use the simpler learning approach and base their decisions on what had been previously rewarded,鈥 adds co-first author Dr Cheke. 鈥淏ut this wasn鈥檛 what we found.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Instead, the researchers showed that information about reward was never a reliable predictor of children鈥檚 choices. Instead, the influence of functionality information increased with age 鈥 by the age of seven, this was the dominant influence in their decision making.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淭his suggests that, remarkably, children begin to emphasise information about physics over information about previous rewards from as young as seven years of age, even when these two types of information are in direct conflict.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This research was funded by the European Research Council under the European Union鈥檚 Seventh Framework Programme.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong>Reference</strong><br />&#13; Elsa Loissel, Lucy Cheke &amp; Nicola Clayton. <a href="https://journals.plos.org:443/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0193264">Exploring the Relative Contributions of Reward-History and Functionality Information to Children鈥檚 Acquisition of The Aesop鈥檚 Fable Task.</a> PLOS ONE; 23 Feb 2018; DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0193264</em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Children as young as seven apply basic laws of physics to problem-solving, rather than learning from what has previously been rewarded, suggests new research from the 国际米兰对阵科莫.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Remarkably, children begin to emphasise information about physics over information about previous rewards from as young as seven years of age, even when these two types of information are in direct conflict</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Lucy Cheke</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="https://www.flickr.com/photos/clairity/2366314902/" target="_blank">Sharon Mollerus</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Dominoes 3</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-license-type field-type-taxonomy-term-reference field-label-above"><div class="field-label">Licence type:&nbsp;</div><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/taxonomy/imagecredit/attribution">Attribution</a></div></div></div> Fri, 23 Feb 2018 19:00:56 +0000 cjb250 195602 at Eye contact with your baby helps synchronise your brainwaves /research/news/eye-contact-with-your-baby-helps-synchronise-your-brainwaves <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/news/ginnylucycrop.jpg?itok=KIIipq0c" alt="Lucy Kivlin and her baby Ginny" title="Lucy Kivlin and her baby Ginny, Credit: None" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>When a parent and infant interact, various aspects of their behaviour can synchronise, including their gaze, emotions and heartrate, but little is known about whether their brain activity also synchronises 鈥 and what the consequences of this might be.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Brainwaves reflect the group-level activity of millions of neurons and are involved in information transfer between brain regions. Previous studies have shown that when two adults are talking to each other, communication is more successful if their brainwaves are in synchrony.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Researchers at the Baby-LINC聽Lab at the 国际米兰对阵科莫 carried out a study to explore whether infants can synchronise their brainwaves to adults too 鈥 and whether eye contact might influence this. Their results are published today in the <em>Proceedings of National Academy of Sciences (PNAS)</em>.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The team examined the brainwave patterns of 36 infants (17 in the first experiment and 19 in the second) using electroencephalography聽(EEG), which measures patterns of brain electrical activity via electrodes in a skull cap worn by the participants. They compared the infants鈥 brain activity to that of the adult who was singing nursery rhymes to the infant.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In the first of two experiments, the infant watched a video of an adult as she sang nursery rhymes. First, the adult 鈥 whose brainwave patterns had already been recorded 鈥 was looking directly at the infant. Then, she turned her head to avert her gaze, while still singing nursery rhymes. Finally, she turned her head away, but her eyes looked directly back at the infant.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>As anticipated, the researchers found that infants鈥 brainwaves were more synchronised to the adults鈥 when the adult鈥檚 gaze met the infant鈥檚, as compared to when her gaze was averted Interestingly, the greatest synchronising effect occurred when the adults鈥 head was turned away but her eyes still looked directly at the infant. The researchers say this may be because such a gaze appears highly deliberate, and so provides a stronger signal to the infant that the adult intends to communicate with her.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In the second experiment, a real adult replaced the video. She only looked either directly at the infant or averted her gaze while singing nursery rhymes. This time, however, her brainwaves could be monitored live to see whether her brainwave patterns were being influenced by the infant鈥檚 as well as the other way round.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This time, both infants and adults became more synchronised to each other鈥檚 brain activity when mutual eye contact was established. This occurred even though the adult could see the infant at all times, and infants were equally interested in looking at the adult even when she looked away. The researchers say that this shows that brainwave synchronisation isn鈥檛 just due to seeing a face or finding something interesting, but about sharing an intention to communicate.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>To measure infants鈥 intention to communicate, the researcher measured how many 鈥榲ocalisations鈥 infants made to the experimenter. As predicted, infants made a greater effort to communicate, making more 鈥榲ocalisations鈥, when the adult made direct eye contact 鈥 and individual infants who made longer vocalisations also had higher brainwave synchrony with the adult.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Victoria Leong, lead author on the study said: 鈥淲hen the adult and infant are looking at each other, they are signalling their availability and intention to communicate with each other. 聽We found that both adult and infant brains respond to a gaze signal by becoming more in sync with their partner. This mechanism could prepare parents and babies to communicate, by synchronising when to speak and when to listen, which would also make learning more effective.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Sam Wass, last author on the study, said: 鈥淲e don鈥檛 know what it is, yet, that causes this synchronous brain activity. We鈥檙e certainly not claiming to have discovered telepathy! In this study, we were looking at whether infants can synchronise their brains to someone else, just as adults can. And we were also trying to figure out what gives rise to the synchrony.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淥ur findings suggested eye gaze and vocalisations may both, somehow, play a role. But the brain synchrony we were observing was at such high time-scales 鈥 of three to nine oscillations per second 鈥 that we still need to figure out how exactly eye gaze and vocalisations create it.鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This research was supported by an ESRC Transformative Research Grant to Dr Leong and Dr Wass.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong>Reference</strong><br />&#13; Leong, V et al. <a href="https://www.pnas.org/doi/abs/10.1073/pnas.1702493114">Speaker gaze increases infant-adult connectivity.</a> PNAS; 28 Nov 2017; DOI: 10.1101/108878</em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Making eye contact with an infant makes adults鈥 and babies鈥 brainwaves 鈥榞et in sync鈥 with each other 鈥 which is likely to support communication and learning 鈥 according to researchers at the 国际米兰对阵科莫.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">When the adult and infant are looking at each other, they are signalling their availability and intention to communicate with each other</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Victoria Leong</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Lucy Kivlin and her baby Ginny</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-panel-title field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Researcher profile: Dr Victoria Leong</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-panel-body field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><img alt="" src="/sites/www.cam.ac.uk/files/inner-images/vl_img_0006.jpg" style="width: 100%; height: 100%;" /></p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Victoria Leong is an Affiliated Lecturer at 国际米兰对阵科莫鈥檚 Department of Psychology, and also an Assistant Professor of Psychology at Nanyang Technological University, Singapore. Her research aims to understand how parents and infants communicate and learn from each other, and the brain mechanisms that help them to interact effectively as social partners.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥淭he Baby-LINC lab is designed to look like a home living room so that mothers and babies feel comfortable,鈥 she says.聽 In the lab, they use a wireless EEG system to measure infants鈥 brain activity, which means that babies don鈥檛 have to be tethered to a computer and we can conduct recordings for longer periods of time. 鈥淭his is invaluable if the baby needs a nap or a nappy change in-between doing our tasks!鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Dr Leong says she is passionate about 鈥渞eal-world neuroscience鈥. In other words, 鈥渦nderstanding and not ignoring the very real 鈥 and often very messy 鈥 human social contexts that infiltrate brain processes鈥. This means that in addition to world class facilities and methods, the ability to collect robust data also depends on keeping the infants relaxed and happy. 鈥淢any a tantrum can be averted by the judicious and timely application of large soapy bubbles and rice cakes. The ability to blow large charming bubbles thereafter became a key criteria for recruiting research assistants!鈥</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The research project came about 鈥渙ver a cup of tea [with Sam Wass] and a notepad to scratch out some frankly outlandish ideas about brain-to-brain synchrony鈥. They received 拢3,995 with the help of 国际米兰对阵科莫 Neuroscience and 国际米兰对阵科莫 Language Sciences for a pilot project and within a year went on to secure an ESRC Transformative Research Grant, which allowed them to significantly scale-up research operations, and to build the first mother-infant EEG hyperscanning facility in the UK (the Baby-LINC Lab).</p>&#13; &#13; <p>鈥湽拭桌级哉罂颇 is one of probably only a handful of highly-creative research environments in the world where young, untested post-doctoral researchers can organically come together, develop ambitious ideas, and have the support to try these out,鈥 she says. 鈥淚 am very proud of our humble beginnings, because they remind me that even a small handful of resources, wisely invested with hard work, can grow into world-class research.鈥</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Wed, 29 Nov 2017 08:29:30 +0000 cjb250 193332 at Opinion: Genetics: what it is that makes you clever 鈥 and why it鈥檚 shrouded in controversy /research/discussion/opinion-genetics-what-it-is-that-makes-you-clever-and-why-its-shrouded-in-controversy <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/discussion/160421reading.jpg?itok=V3W4ExFT" alt="" title="Credit: None" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>For nearly 150 years, the concept of intelligence and its study have offered scientific ways of classifying people in terms of their 鈥渁bility鈥. The drive to identify and quantify exceptional mental capacity may have a chequered <a href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hast.499/abstract;jsessionid=1C167A1612F22CDFE6340960AC893439.f04t03?userIsAuthenticated=false&amp;amp;deniedAccessCustomisedMessage=">history</a>, but it is still being pursued by some researchers today.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Francis Galton, who was Charles Darwin鈥檚 cousin, is considered the father of eugenics and was one of the first to formally study intelligence. His 1869 work <a href="https://books.google.co.uk/books/about/Hereditary_Genius.html?id=1h0Ztc1q-RoC&amp;source=kp_cover&amp;redir_esc=y">Hereditary Genius</a> argued that superior mental capabilities were passed down via natural selection 鈥 confined to Europe鈥檚 most eminent men, a 鈥渓ineage of genius鈥. Barring a few exceptions, women, ethnic minorities and lower socioeconomic communities were labelled as inferior in intelligence.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Galton鈥檚 controversial theories on race, socioeconomics and intelligence have been highly influential and shaped the ideologies of numerous researchers and theorists around the world.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>In the UK, proponents of a Galtonian view on intelligence included educational psychologist <a href="https://www.timeshighereducation.com/books/a-true-pro-and-his-cons/161397.article">Cyril Burt</a>, who helped formulate the 11-plus examination, and <a href="https://www.britannica.com:443/biography/Charles-E-Spearman">psychologist Charles Spearman</a> who is best known for his creation of the concept 鈥済鈥 鈥 the innate general factor of human mental ability. Spearman鈥檚 background as an engineer in the British army gave him a statistical sophistication that proved instrumental in shifting the direction of the field of intelligence study.</p>&#13; &#13; <figure class="align-right "><img alt="" src="https://62e528761d0685343e1c-f3d1b99a743ffa4142d9d7f1978d9686.ssl.cf2.rackcdn.com/files/115800/width237/image-20160321-30917-1i9hs6m.jpg" style="width: 100%;" /><figcaption><span class="caption">Spearman: statistician who delved into human intelligence.</span> <span class="attribution"><a class="source" href="https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File%3AExposition_universelle_de_1900_-_portraits_des_commissaires_g%C3%A9n%C3%A9raux-Charles_Spearman.jpg">Eug猫ne Pirou via Wikimedia Commons</a></span></figcaption></figure><p>聽</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Spearman hypothesised that intelligence is comprised of 鈥<a href="https://www.jstor.org/stable/1412107?origin=crossref&amp;amp;seq=1">g</a>鈥 鈥 or 鈥済eneral intelligence鈥, and two other specific factors: verbal ability and fluency. Spearman鈥檚 extensive work on the use of 鈥済鈥 within the field of statistics meant that some used the 鈥渉ard鈥 sciences and maths as instruments to argue that there were biological differences between races and social classes. 鈥淕鈥 as a representation of the biological basis of intelligence <a href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hast.494/abstract">is still being used today in research</a> within the current field of behavioural genetics.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Political currency</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>The concept of inheritance, and specifically the inheritance of intelligence, has carried over into political and educational spheres. A more recent advocate of Galtonian-inspired ideas is Dominic Cummings, who served as a special advisor to the former secretary of state for education, Michael Gove. Cummings wrote the following in a <a href="http://s3.documentcloud.org/documents/804396/some-thoughts-on-education-and-political.pdf">237-page document</a> titled 鈥淪ome thoughts on education and political priorities鈥:</p>&#13; &#13; <p>聽</p>&#13; &#13; <blockquote>&#13; <p>Raising school performance of poorer children 鈥 would not necessarily lower parent-offspring correlations (nor change heritability estimates). When people look at the gaps between rich and poor children that already exist at a young age (3-5), they almost universally assume that these differences are because of environmental reasons (鈥減rivileges of wealth鈥) and ignore genetics.</p>&#13; </blockquote>&#13; &#13; <h2>聽</h2>&#13; &#13; <h2>The birth of twins studies</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>From the 1920s, when <a href="https://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08856559.1932.10533098?journalCode=vzpg20">twin and adoption studies</a> set out to determine the genetic and environmental origins of intelligence differences, the study of intelligence began to converse with the early stages of human behavioural genetics.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Under the presumption that twins experience similar environmental aspects, <a href="https://link.springer.com/article/10.1023/A:1001959306025">twins studies enable researchers</a> to evaluate the variance of a given outcome 鈥 such as cognitive ability 鈥 in a large group. They can then attempt to estimate how much of this variance is due to the heritability of genes, the shared environment the twins live in, or a non-shared environment.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The 1980s and 1990s saw another rise in twin and adoption studies on intelligence, many of which were more systematic in nature due to advances in technology. Most supported earlier research and showed intelligence to be highly heritable and polygenic, meaning that it is influenced by many different genetic markers.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>The researchers <a href="https://kclpure.kcl.ac.uk/portal/en/publications/multivariate-behavioral-genetics-and-development-twin-studies%28f51376fe-96e6-4288-811f-9b44cead12c9%29.html">Robert Plomin</a>, <a href="https://www.annualreviews.org/content/journals/10.1146/annurev.ps.29.020178.002353">JC Defries</a>, and <a href="https://link.springer.com/article/10.1023/A:1010257512183">Nele Jacobs</a> were at the forefront of this new wave of studies. But this research was still unable to identify the specific genetic markers within the human genome that are connected to intelligence.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Genome 鈥 a new frontier</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>Genome sequencing technologies have taken the search for the genetic components of inheritance another step forward. Despite the seemingly endless possibilities brought forth by the <a href="https://www.genome.gov/12011239">Human Genome Project in 2001</a>, actually using DNA-based techniques to locate which genetic differences contribute to observed differences in intelligence <a href="https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1002/hast.496/abstract">has been markedly more difficult</a> than anticipated.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Genome-wide association studies (GWAS) began to take hold as a powerful tool for investigating the human genetic architecture. These studies assess connections between a trait and a multitude of DNA markers. Most commonly, they look for single-nucleotide polymorphisms, or SNPs. These are variations between genes at specific locations throughout a DNA sequence that might determine an individual鈥檚 likelihood to develop a particular disease or trait.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Originally intended to identify genetic risk factors associated with <a href="https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1109557">susceptibility to disease</a>, GWAS have become a means through which to try and pinpoint the genetic factors responsible <a href="https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2012.184">for cognitive ability</a>. But researchers have <a href="https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/science.1235488">shown</a> that intelligence is a trait influenced by many different genes: they have so far been unable to locate enough SNPs to predict the IQ of an individual.</p>&#13; &#13; <h2>Ethical questions</h2>&#13; &#13; <p>There鈥檚 a long way still to go, but this field is receiving <a href="https://www.telegraph.co.uk/education/educationnews/11680895/Children-should-be-genetically-screened-at-the-age-of-4-to-aid-their-education-expert-claims.html">a great deal of publicity</a>. This raises several ethical questions. We must ask ourselves if this research can ever be socially neutral given the eugenic-Galtonian history underpinning it.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>This kind of research could have an impact on <a href="https://nautil.us/super_intelligent-humans-are-coming-235110/">human genetic engineering</a> and the choices parents make when deciding to have children. It could give parents with the money and desire to do so the option to make their offspring 鈥渟marter鈥. Though genetically engineering intelligence may appear to be in the realm of science fiction, if the genes associated with intelligence are identified, it could become a reality.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Some <a href="https://www.wiley.com/en-gb/G+is+for+Genes%3A+The+Impact+of+Genetics+on+Education+and+Achievement-p-9781118482780">researchers</a> have suggested that schools which have a child鈥檚 genetic information could tailor the curriculum and teaching to create a system of 鈥減ersonalised learning鈥. But this could lead schools to expect certain levels of achievement from certain groups of children 鈥 perhaps from different socioeconomic or ethnic groups 鈥 and would raise questions of whether richer families would benefit most.</p>&#13; &#13; <p>Whether calling it 鈥渋ntelligence鈥, 鈥渃ognitive ability鈥, or 鈥淚Q鈥, behavioural genetics research is still trying to identify the genetic markers for a trait that can predict, in essence, a person鈥檚 success in life. Given the history of this field of research, it鈥檚 vital it is conducted with an awareness of its possible ethical impact on all parts of society.</p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong><span><a href="https://theconversation.com/profiles/daphne-martschenko-238687">Daphne Martschenko</a>, PhD Candidate, <a href="https://theconversation.com/institutions/university-of-cambridge-1283">国际米兰对阵科莫</a></span></strong></em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em><strong>This article was originally published on <a href="https://theconversation.com/">The Conversation</a>. Read the <a href="https://theconversation.com/genetics-what-it-is-that-makes-you-clever-and-why-its-shrouded-in-controversy-56115">original article</a>.</strong></em></p>&#13; &#13; <p><em>The opinions expressed in this article are those of the individual author(s) and do not represent the views of the 国际米兰对阵科莫.</em></p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Daphne Martschenko (Faculty of Education) discusses the concept of intelligence and the drive to identify and quantify it.</p>&#13; </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br />&#13; The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p>&#13; </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Thu, 21 Apr 2016 11:00:00 +0000 Anonymous 171822 at Education and the brain: what happens when children learn? /research/features/education-and-the-brain-what-happens-when-children-learn <div class="field field-name-field-news-image field-type-image field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><img class="cam-scale-with-grid" src="/sites/default/files/styles/content-580x288/public/news/research/features/160205frontallobecreditbodyparts3danatomography.jpg?itok=-Q39C9tc" alt="Frontal lobe" title="Frontal lobe, Credit: BodyParts3D/Anatomography" /></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-body field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p>Researchers looking at child development often use search-and-find tasks to look at the ways in which children apply what they are learning about the physical world. Tests carried out on toddlers reveal that something quite remarkable happens in child development between the ages of two and five 鈥 a stage identified by both educationalists and neuroscientists as critical to the capacity for learning.</p> <p>Dr Sara Baker is a researcher into early childhood at the Faculty of Education. She is interested in the role of the brain鈥檚 prefrontal lobe in how young children learn to adapt their understanding to an ever-shifting environment. Many of her studies chart changes in children鈥檚 ways of thinking about the world. She uses longitudinal designs to examine the shape of individual children鈥檚 learning curves month by month.</p> <p>Research by Baker and colleagues is contributing to an understanding of the acquisition of skills essential to learning. She explains: 鈥淭he brain鈥檚 frontal lobe is one of the four major divisions of the cerebral cortex. It regulates decision-making, problem-solving and behaviour. We call these functions executive skills 鈥 they are at the root of the cognitive differences between humans and other animals. My executive functions enable me to resist a slice of cake when I know I鈥檓 soon having dinner.鈥</p> <p>In an experiment designed to identify the age at which executive skills develop, Baker and colleagues used a row of four interconnected boxes to test children鈥檚 ability to apply their knowledge of basic physics. A ball rolled down an incline entered the first box and disappeared. A barrier (its top visible) was slotted in between two of the boxes to stop the ball rolling any further. The children were asked to open the door of the box in which the ball was hidden.</p> <p>Aged 29鈥31 months, only 32% of the children correctly identified the location of the ball by working out that the barrier would have stopped it. Aged 32鈥36 months, 66% of children were successful. Toddlers under the age of three appear to understand the principles of solidity and continuity, but have trouble acting on this knowledge. A single month in a child鈥檚 age affected their ability to carry out the task correctly.</p> <p>Baker鈥檚 interest in children鈥檚 development of executive skills dates from the moment a decade ago when she picked up a picture book while sitting in the foyer of a nursery school; the narrative focused on opposites: big/small, light/dark, hot/cold. How would children respond if they were asked to point to the opposite picture to the one depicting the word they heard spoken? This question became the topic for her PhD. Her findings confirmed that the huge variability of children鈥檚 executive skills could explain the range of social and cognitive behaviours we see emerging in the early years. What we learn at this stage, and what we learn to apply, sets us on course for life.</p> <p>Most three-year-olds find the 鈥榦pposites鈥 task hard. Given two pictures of bears, one big, one small, they automatically point to the big bear when they hear the word 鈥榖ig鈥 spoken aloud. They point to the big bear even when they have been asked (and appear to have understood) to point to the image that is the opposite of the word they hear.</p> <p>Five-year-olds are much more successful in carrying out the task explained to them. 鈥淏y age five, most children have acquired the ability to override their impulses, and put them on hold, in order to follow a request,鈥 says Baker. 鈥淭he ability to control impulses is vital to children鈥檚 socialisation, their ability to share and work in groups 鈥 and ultimately to be adaptable and well adjusted.鈥</p> <p>What happens in children鈥檚 brains and minds to enable them to make these important leaps in understanding? The answer involves an understanding of neuroscience as well as child development. Baker and colleagues are engaged in multidisciplinary projects including examining how individuals with autism may perceive and learn about the physical world differently from those without a diagnosis. Her team is also developing a pedagogical, play-based approach in collaboration with teachers.</p> <p>鈥淓xecutive function is a hot topic in education. When we talk to teachers about the psychology behind frontal lobe development, they immediately recognise how important self-regulation is, and will tell you about the child who can鈥檛 concentrate. It might be the case that this child is struggling with their executive functions: their working memory or inhibitory control might be flagging,鈥 says Baker.</p> <p>鈥淭he tricky part is to grasp the processes developing in the child鈥檚 brain and come up with ways to encourage that development. In early years鈥 education, playful learning and giving children freedom to explore could help to encourage independence as well as the ability to know when to ask for help, both of which depend on self-regulatory skills. If we want to encourage adaptability and self-reliance, we have to look beyond the formal curriculum.鈥</p> <p>Baker鈥檚 research into children鈥檚 ability to apply knowledge to successfully predict the location of an object hidden from view revealed much more than simply which age group was successful. She says: 鈥淚n looking at the data from tasks, it鈥檚 not enough to focus only on children鈥檚 failures. We need to look at why they search for an object in a particular place. Often they鈥檙e applying something else that they鈥檝e learnt.鈥</p> <p>When younger children opened the same door twice in the boxes experiment, despite the barrier having been moved, they were applying logic: an object may be precisely where it was found before. After all, it鈥檚 always worth looking for the house keys first where they should be.</p> <p>In another experiment (involving dropping balls into opaque tubes that crossed each other), the younger children applied their knowledge of gravity (the ball would fall down the tube) but failed to take into account that the tubes were not straight. Baker says: 鈥淲hen children repeat a mistake, they reveal something about their view of the world and, as researchers, we learn how their brain is developing. As teachers and parents, our role is to help children to overcome that strong, but wrong, impulse.鈥</p> <p>During the course of a day, your frontal lobe will have enabled you to do far more than find your keys. The synaptic firing of millions of cells in your brain may have guided you through a tricky situation with colleagues or prompted you to make a split-second decision as you crossed a busy road. 鈥淭he development of this vital area of your brain happened well before you started formal education and will continue throughout your lifetime,鈥 says Baker.</p> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-summary field-type-text-with-summary field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><p>Have you lost your house keys recently? If so, you probably applied a spot of logical thinking. You looked first in the most obvious places 鈥 bags and pockets 鈥 and then mentally retraced your steps to the point when you last used them.</p> </p></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">The tricky part is to grasp the processes developing in the child鈥檚 brain and come up with ways to encourage that development.</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-content-quote-name field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Sara Baker</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-credit field-type-link-field field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><a href="/" target="_blank">BodyParts3D/Anatomography</a></div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-image-desctiprion field-type-text field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Frontal lobe</div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-cc-attribute-text field-type-text-long field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even"><p><a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license"><img alt="Creative Commons License" src="https://i.creativecommons.org/l/by/4.0/88x31.png" style="border-width:0" /></a><br /> The text in this work is licensed under a <a href="http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/" rel="license">Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License</a>. For image use please see separate credits above.</p> </div></div></div><div class="field field-name-field-show-cc-text field-type-list-boolean field-label-hidden"><div class="field-items"><div class="field-item even">Yes</div></div></div> Wed, 10 Feb 2016 12:27:02 +0000 amb206 166882 at